Mutual Aid. Pablo Servigne

Mutual Aid - Pablo  Servigne


Скачать книгу
role of environmental conditions in the evolution of mutual aid: in a hostile climate (cold, drought, etc.), mutual aid is a significant advantage in terms of survival.

      A second reason why Kropotkin found it easier to observe mutual aid than Darwin did probably derived from his culture. Educated in the humanist values of the Enlightenment, he then travelled extensively in Western Europe in contact with the working class, which was developing a culture of solidarity and association. In addition, Kropotkin, who had readers in the Western world (he was published in English and French), had a dual scientific culture: he had also been trained in the Russian school of zoology, represented by Karl Fedorovich Kessler (1815–81), who shared Darwin’s ideas of evolution and selection, but not his Malthusian worldview.

      The scientific community, gradually won over to the West’s competitive representation of the world,64 forgot Kropotkin. His ‘biased’ view of a cooperative nature did not mesh with that of modern evolutionary biology, predominantly English-speaking, imbued with anti-communism, and working more and more on genes and individuals (selfish, of course).65 Few have tried to rescue Kropotkin from limbo. But it is worth remembering a work published in 1988 by the brilliant palaeontologist Stephen Jay Gould.66 Today, the few scientists who cite Kropotkin do so with a mixture of embarrassment, condescension and fascination: his book is remarkable but uncritical,67 it is ‘wonderfully well written’, but ‘Kropotkin tells of seeing animal cooperation (that is, mutual aid) at every turn’;68 it’s a fascinating book, challenging as it does the predominance of the ‘struggle for life’ paradigm,69 presenting ‘an unapologetically positivist and biased view of the natural world’.70

       The life, death and rebirth of sociobiology, 1970–2000

      In the mid-1970s, Edward O. Wilson, a brilliant professor at Harvard University specializing in social insects, in particular ants, created a new scientific discipline: sociobiology.71 He proposed to synthesize everything that is known about animal societies using methods derived from ethology, genetics and the science of evolution. In his book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis, published in 1975, he created an impressive compilation of the latest experimental knowledge on the subject,72 but also cemented his point with a simple mathematical equation borrowed from a young British student, William Hamilton, that supposedly explained the evolution of all social behaviour. A bold move!

      Hamilton’s equation refers to genetics and describes a very simple idea: an individual has genes in common with its fellows (especially its family); so, by helping one of its relatives (i.e. to survive and reproduce), it participates indirectly in the transmission of a proportion of its own genes. In other words, I have more interest in sacrificing myself for my brother (who has many of my genes) than in sacrificing myself for a complete stranger. So, according to the theory,73 ‘altruistic genes’ spread via mutual aid among relatives.

      In the following years, generations of sociobiologists therefore endeavoured to highlight genetic correlations between organisms that cooperated. The prediction was that the closer individuals were genetically, the more they would help each other. In the initial euphoria, believing that there was a universal equation (one that was diabolically simple, and therefore beautiful), many researchers pulled the argument even further, and used it to explain altruistic human behaviour by genetic proximity. Wilson was the first, in the famous chapter 23 of his Sociobiology – the one devoted to humans – to suggest that human altruistic behaviour could be explained by the fact that apparently altruistic individuals are in reality simply helping their loved ones to reproduce more easily. This was not just a misleadingly simplistic and deterministic vision; above all, it was devoid of any experimental proof.

      The argument also pulled some people in another direction: they emphasized the ‘natural’ preference for one’s family and race. Sociobiological theory gave grist to the mill of those who thought that altruism must be correlated with genetic proximity. This encouraged certain ideologues, grouped under the label of the ‘New Right’, to justify racism. On the opposite side of the political spectrum, the left, the critiques were of course vociferous, in particular on the part of the social sciences, but also in Marxist circles, which had always had a horror of biological determinism.

      So the 2000s began with this paradox: sociobiology, which considered nature as fundamentally selfish, produced an increasing number of studies of altruism and cooperation – and this ultimately resulted in some extraordinary discoveries.

       The renaissance of the 2000s

      There has been a real paradigm shift in sociobiology. Its founding father, Edward O. Wilson, with the help of excellent theorists and in the light of new discoveries in the field and the laboratory, recently ‘flipped’ his theory by reversing its basic assumption. The origin of the ‘social fact’ is no longer to be found in genes, but in the influence of the environment. From then on, some scientists began to recognize the importance of Kropotkin’s work, especially as he highlighted the importance of the environment in the evolution of mutual aid.

      Thanks to this abundance of new studies, we can finally see the complexity of the general picture of mutual aid and gradually start to weave connections between disciplines. Among the most significant advances, let us cite the pioneering work of the experimental economist Ernst Fehr, the discoveries on primates made by Frans de Waal, Michael Tomasello and Felix Warneken, political scientist Elinor Ostrom’s surprising findings on ‘the commons’, and the advances in neuroscience made by Tania Singer and Jean Decety, not forgetting the investigations into comparative anthropology conducted by Joe Heinrich in other human cultures. Even genetics has become acceptable again thanks to the remarkable mechanisms of epigenetics, which show how the environment and culture play an important role in gene expression.

      We will be exploring all of this in the following chapters.

      1 1. R. M. Callaway and L. R. Walker, ‘Competition and facilitation: A synthetic approach to interactions in plant communities’, Ecology, 78, 1997, pp. 1958–65.

      2 2.


Скачать книгу