Health Psychology. Michael Murray

Health Psychology - Michael  Murray


Скачать книгу

      1 It must contain a sensor that measures the value of the regulated variable.

      2 It must contain a mechanism for establishing the ‘normal range’ of values for the regulated variable. In the model shown in Figure 2.13, this mechanism is represented by the ‘Set point Y’. Arguably, the term ‘set range’ would be more appropriate than ‘set point’.

      3 It must contain an ‘error detector’ that compares the signal being transmitted by the sensor (representing the actual value of the regulated variable) with the set point or range. The result of this comparison is an error signal that is interpreted by the controller.

      4 The controller interprets the error signal and determines the value of the outputs of the effectors. In the vast majority of cases, the controller is an automatic, nonconscious process.

      5 The effectors are those elements that determine the value of the regulated variable.

      Figure 2.13 A complete representation of physiological homeostatic mechanisms (adapted from Modell et al., 2015)

      We turn next to consider psychological homeostasis. Identical principles to those described above for physiological homeostasis apply to the regulation of behaviour and experience (Figure 2.14). For psychological homeostasis, however, the internal effectors remain active but the boundary between the internal and external environments lies between the controller and the outward effectors of the somatic nervous system, i.e., the muscles that control speech and action.

      Figure 2.14 A complete representation of psychological homeostatic mechanisms (adapted from Modell et al., 2015)

      Let us consider how psychological homeostasis works in practice. All people are oriented towards seeking and/or preserving physical and subjective well-being at a set point that is kept at the highest possible level. Hence, we tend to approach new resources in the hope of finding a reward for this behaviour and equally to avoid punishing or confrontational situations. If we do encounter threat, our behavioural options include either ‘fight or flight’ or inhibiting our behaviour so as to go unnoticed and to avoid confrontation. One can easily imagine the adaptive value of behavioural inhibition. A mouse scurrying through the grass suddenly notices a buzzard flying overhead. Out of fear, the mouse freezes, thus avoiding attracting the buzzard’s attention. Playing dead until a predator has passed can be beneficial, as long as the tension of waiting does not have to go on for too long. Figure 2.15 shows a homeostatic strategy for choosing optimal behaviours and the brain structures that may be involved in the process. The diagram shows the feedback loops whereby our memories associate positive or negative connotations with situations that we experience, and then guide behaviour the next time they arise.

      Figure 2.15 Approach and avoidance behaviours that maximize well-being

      Source: Copyleft, http://thebrain.mcgill.ca

      Life isn’t always this simple, however. The whole process could fall apart if you’re not a mouse hiding from a buzzard but, for example, you’re a worker dealing with an exploitative boss. The worker cannot fight or flee, or they would be out of a job. So they can either join a labour union and talk to the union representative or they can let months and years go by while they inhibit their behaviour. This ‘do-nothing’ strategy ultimately can have disastrous effects on their health. For one thing, such inhibition causes hormonal changes that produce high blood levels of glucocorticoids, whose depressive effect on IS function is well known. This weakening of the IS is why remaining in a prolonged state of behavioural inhibition can cause all kinds of health problems.

      One source of inhibition is our imagination – our fear of failure. This can lead us to foresee so many potentially negative scenarios that we end up doing nothing. To do nothing, and to maintain a dream, may be a better option than to act and to fall flat on one’s face. Whichever way one looks at the issue of inhibition, it has an obvious connection with homeostasis, a striving towards equilibrium.

      Initiated by the brain, homeostasis also can act in an anticipatory mode. The preprandial (prior to having a meal) secretion of insulin, ghrelin and other hormones enables the consumption of a larger nutrient load with only minimal postprandial homeostatic consequences. When a meal containing carbohydrates is to be consumed, a variety of hormones is secreted by the gut that elicit the secretion of insulin from the pancreas before the blood sugar level has actually started to rise. This starts lowering the blood sugar level in anticipation of the influx of large quantities of glucose from the gut into the blood. This has the effect of blunting the blood glucose concentration spike that would otherwise occur. The relevance of psychological homeostasis has been underestimated in the study of behaviour, health and illness. In this book, psychological homeostasis is given its rightful position at the ‘hightable’ of psychological constructs.

      Quack’s corner and the placebo effect

      The placebo effect can be viewed as a form of anticipatory homeostasis. When a patient feels unwell, for example with depression, and seeks help from a trusted medical practitioner, (s)he is given a prescription. Upon receiving the medicine, (s)he anticipates feeling better and, after swallowing the allegedly curative pill, sits back and looks for the signs of increased well-being and improvement. Lo and behold, in many such cases (s)he actually does feel better, and her/his well-being is restored. If it is an active placebo with somatic side effects, then even better (Thomson, 1982). To the degree that any treatment is perceived to be effective, then expectancy must play a role. Homeopathic medicine, which has no active content by definition, provides a cogent example of anticipatory homeostasis with no harmful side effects. Homeopathic medicine is a 100% placebo masquerading as active medicine. Yet this form of medicine is used worldwide by hundreds of millions of people who swear by its efficacy.

      Homeopathy has two principal ‘laws’, the first being ‘similia similibus curentur’ or ‘let likes be cured by likes’. This ‘law’ means that treatments that cause specific symptoms (e.g., onions cause runny eyes and nose) can cure conditions that cause the same symptoms (e.g., a bad cold). As if that isn’t kooky enough, there is the ‘law of infinitesimal doses’ that when treatments are diluted in water or alcohol, they actually increase in therapeutic potency. This means that a 1-in-1,000 solution should be more effective than a 1-in-100 dilution. It’s an inverted dose response curve.

      The anticipatory phenomena of placebos are certainly not confined to complementary and alternative medicines. The placebo effect has wide application in all areas of medicine. There is no harm in it whatsoever, as long as one doesn’t swallow the quack claims that come along as part of the package with the pills.

      Consider antidepressants. These are supposed to work by fixing a chemical imbalance, specifically, a lack of serotonin in the brain. Irving Kirsch (2015) reviews analyses of published and unpublished data that were hidden by drug companies, revealing that:

      most (if not all) of the benefits are due to the placebo effect. Some antidepressants increase serotonin levels, some decrease it, and some have no effect at all on serotonin. Nevertheless, they all show the same therapeutic benefit. Even the small statistical difference between antidepressants and placebos may be an enhanced placebo effect, due to the fact that most patients and doctors in clinical trials successfully break blind. The serotonin theory is as close as any theory in the history of science to having been proved wrong. Instead of curing depression, popular antidepressants may induce a biological vulnerability making people more likely to become depressed in the future. (Kirsch, 2015: 128, italics added)

      Whatever people may believe will help them to feel better may indeed help them to feel better, at least for a short while. Long-term, however, placebo effects almost always fade. This isn’t being snootily cynical.


Скачать книгу