A Companion to Chomsky. Группа авторов

A Companion to Chomsky - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
plausibly do respect UG, requires careful analyses of the expressions in these languages – just as appreciating the underlying unities in any domain requires analyses beyond surface appearances (as in biology, chemistry, physics – who would have thought that ice and steam are the same substance?).

      The present section covers multiple topics that all relate to contemporary issues in syntax. In Chapter 8, David Adger deals with the nature of hierarchical relations, in particular the computational procedure needed to generate such relations. In contemporary parlance, this operation is called Merge, and it has become a hallmark of the program, partly also because of Chomsky's suggestion that Merge may be one of the evolutionary novelties that enabled humans to develop language. Adger is concerned with what kind of computing device this is, and among others, he discusses the relationship between the abstract specification of a function and procedures to compute it.

      As we've mentioned, language variation has always been important to Chomsky's program, leading to the emergence of the Principles and Parameters theory in the late 1970s. In Chapter 10, Baker discusses the importance of linguistic diversity in Chomsky's work, as described above. Baker divides Chomsky's career into three different phases, using three influential books as representatives of each phase.

      In Chapter 11, Michelle Sheehan probes the issue further in discussing Chomsky's influence on the modelling of linguistic variation, in particular work that has suggested some kind of parametric approach to language variation. Originally, this approach assumed that linguistic variation was pretty much like a “switchboard” where the task of acquiring syntax was confined to determining the correct setting for each property listed on the switchboard. This model has undergone substantial changes, which Sheehan discusses alongside objections to the very concept of parameters.

      As Cheng and Griffiths' chapter in Part I made clear, one of the enduring discoveries of the Chomskyan research program is the restrictions it proposed on non‐local dependencies, such as those involved in wh‐movement, where, as we saw above, the movement of wh‐ words out of a conjunction is barred (see also related examples (a)–(c) in fn. 4). Gereon Müller's chapter (Chapter 12) discusses these in great detail and provides an overview of the various ways in which these have been accounted for.

      Chomsky's own work has mostly focused on synchronic grammatical analyses. However, his work has also inspired work on historical change mainly in the area of syntax, in particular through the realization that much language change happens through language acquisition. Chapter 13, Elly van Gelderen's chapter, provides an overview of this development, including his most recent work on labeling.

      Another area where Chomsky's thinking has inspired a great deal of work is the area of second language acquisition. In Chapter 14, Roumyana Slabakova discusses ways in which work on second language acquisition has embraced theoretical developments in syntax, and how it has sometimes informed those developments as well. In recent years, work on multilingual speakers more generally has burgeoned. This research has tried to unravel the nature of mental representations in speakers who master multiple languages. Chapter 15 by Tanja Kupisch, Sergio Miguel Pereira Soares, Eloi Puig‐Mayenco, and Jason Rothman puts this recent development in context and explains its importance.

      As we noted at the beginning of this introduction,another approach that is often assumed to be incompatible with Chomsky's approach is statistical learning in its many different guises. In Chapter 17, Lisa Pearl argues that this is too simplistic, and that statistical learning can both complement UG and help refine our models of its contents. Her focus is in particular on how statistical modeling provides a better understanding of how learners navigate the hypothesis space they are faced with, and she argues that within an approach that remains generative, statistical learning might even replace UG as an explanation for at least certain properties of the adult grammar and how they are acquired.

      The last chapter in this section, Chapter 18, written by Frederick Newmeyer, scrutinizes differences between Chomsky's linguistics and what can broadly be labeled “usage‐based linguistics.” The latter is an umbrella term for a variety of approaches that share the commitment to study the use of language as opposed to focusing on the underlying computational systems of syntax that enable such use. This includes “constructionist” work that sees the basic units of language as grammatical constructions (e.g. active and passive) and the “functionalist” perspective that regards the use of language (primarily in communication) as a major influence on grammar. Comparing Chomsky's work to the shared commitments of usage‐based linguistics is complicated by the fact that Chomsky has rarely engaged with published work in this tradition. However, Newmeyer solves this problem by selecting what he takes to be the most important issues separating the two traditions and carefully outlining the positions that they have taken on those issues.


Скачать книгу