The Behavior of Animals. Группа авторов

The Behavior of Animals - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
that a feature A combined with a feature B may provide a certain sign-stimulus, but that feature A in combination with a feature C may provide a different sign-stimulus. For example, in male sticklebacks:

       red belly and head-down posture addresses a threat signal to conspecific males, but not to females;

       red belly and zigzag dance (Chapter 3) addresses a courtship signal to conspecific females, but not to males.

      It is the combination—configuration or “Gestalt” (e.g., see Koffka 1922)—of behaviorally relevant features that determines the releasing value of a sign-stimulus in the sense of a “stimulus-pattern.” Its perception requires pattern recognition—a process, in which genetic and/or learning factors can be involved.

      A configuration is perceived as the whole. This means that the sum of the responses to the features, when each feature is presented alone, is significantly less than the response to the complete array. Furthermore, recognition is independent of certain changes in other stimulus parameters as long as these do not affect the configuration. This phenomenon is called invariance.

      Sign-stimuli provide parsimonious ways of encoding information to release adequately motivated behaviors. They also have survival value, since they are recognized quickly and responded to rapidly and unambigiously. With these attributes in mind, we continue to use the term sign-stimulus. Its efficacy can be analyzed in experiments using dummies by changing, omitting, adding, or exaggerating certain features.

      Sign-stimuli allow humans to communicate with other animals. The wildlife biologist Kent Clegg used his ultralight aircraft as a sign-stimulus for captive-bred endangered whooping cranes, Grus americanus. Simulating their parents, he painted the wings of the plane white with black tips and thus instructed the young cranes to fly and follow the small aircraft. After leaving Idaho and making three overnight stops, he succeeded in having the young follow the plane on their first migratory trip at 35 mph—matching crane’s flight-speed—for 800 miles to their winter residence in New Mexico (see also https://friendsofthewildwhoopers.org/whooping-cranes-facts-management [accessed: 08/11/20]).

      Principle of configurational sign-stimuli: picking out visual key features

       Static and dynamic configurations

      An example of dynamic configuration concerns the goose/hawk discrimination. Tinbergen (1951) showed that a bird model (Figure 2.3c) elicited escape in young turkeys, Gallopavo meleagris, when the model was flown overhead with the short end and the wings leading, simulating the silhouette of an airborne bird of prey. But the same model flown with the long end leading, resembling a harmless goose-like bird, was ignored. Subsequent research has shown that this configurational discrimination resulted from stimulus-specific habituation discussed later in this chapter.

       Are there comparable signs with threatening stimuli across species?

      Common toads, Bufo bufo, interpret small elongated objects—a worm or millipede—as prey. Experimentally, it can be shown that a 2.5 × 40 mm stripe oriented parallel to the direction of its movement releases eager prey-catching (see Further Reading, Movie A2). The same stripe suddenly oriented crosswise to the direction of movement—in a split-second—leads the toad to “freeze.” That configuration signals threat. Prey-like again, the stripe immediately elicits strong prey-catching activity, etc. The discrimination of these dynamic configurational “opposite stimuli” is invariant to changes in movement direction (Figures 2.3d, 2.4a–d) and velocity. This phenomenon was also observed in terrestrial urodeles (Finkenstädt & Ewert 1983).

      Interestingly, the mudskipper Periophthalmus barbarus, an amphibious fish (Burghagen in Kutschera et al. 2008), and the preying insect Sphodromantis lineola (Kral & Prete 2004; cit., 2004) respond to such test-stripes in the same way as toads. Toward the threat-configuration, mudskippers may even raise their dorsal fins, thus threatening back.

      Threatening postures, as components of agonistic behavior, shown for stickleback (Figure 2.1A), perch (Heiligenberg et al. 1972), or great blue heron (Figure 2.1B), are widespread in the animal kingdom. The caterpillar of Hemeroplanes triptolemus, if attacked, displays a scaring snake-like posture that is disregarded as prey. If human divers encounter a shark, experts recommend assuming an erect posture that will not fit shark’s prey schema.

       Configurational Stimuli in Human Perception

      Configurational perception allows one to extract a figure from background. This is not feasible if both figure and ground are composed of similar items: the figure is masked like a needle in a haystack. However, if items of the figure differ from the background in one aspect, say if they move coherently, the figure isolates. When an observer moves in front of a patterned stationary three-dimensional landscape, nearby objects move faster than those farther away and isolate from each other as flowers, bushes, trees, hills, etc.


Скачать книгу