The Behavior of Animals. Группа авторов

The Behavior of Animals - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
in the section on displacement activities.

      Central versus peripheral locus of action

      A fourth pervasive issue in motivation concerns the locus of action of causal factors. Do causal factors operate within the central nervous system (CNS) or at a more peripheral level? Once again, common sense suggests that they must act in both places; nonetheless, this has also been a controversial issue. Historically, the controversy arose as a reaction by the early behaviorist school in psychology to the views of the introspectionists, who thought one could understand behavior by reflecting on one’s own experiences (see Chapter 1). The behaviorists were skeptical of internal causes that could not be investigated directly, and they attempted to explain as much behavior as possible in terms of stimuli and responses that could be measured physically. However, as it has become more and more possible to measure and manipulate events that occur within the CNS, one major objection to the postulation of central factors has been removed. Nonetheless, some researchers continue to emphasize central or peripheral factors.

      Causal Factors

      Stimuli

      Stimuli can control behavior in many ways: they can release, direct, inhibit, and prime behavior. Chapter 2 discussed many examples of stimuli that release and direct various behavior patterns. Some stimuli can have exactly the opposite effect: rather than facilitate behavior, they inhibit it. A good example is provided by the nest-building behavior of many species of birds. Birds typically build their nests using specific behavior patterns. The stimuli that release and direct their behavior have been studied in several cases and conform to the general principles already discussed. However, at a certain point the birds stop building and no longer react to the twigs, lichens, or feathers with which they construct their nest. There are several possible reasons why they stop, but one reason is that the stimuli provided by the completed nest inhibit further nest building. This can be seen when a bird takes over a complete nest from the previous season and shows very little nest-building behavior. Other birds, in the same internal state, that have not found an old nest show a great deal of nest-building behavior (Thorpe 1956).

      Figure 3.4 Courtship and mating behavior of the three-spined stickleback. The male is on the left and the female, with a swollen belly, is on the right. A typical courtship sequence is indicated below the diagram. (From Tinbergen 1951).

      These examples of priming all occur during the time span of a few minutes. Some stimuli prime behavior over a much longer period. Stimuli from the eggs of the stickleback inhibit sexual behavior, as we have just seen, but they also prime parental behavior. Male sticklebacks fan the eggs in their nest by moving their fins in a characteristic manner, which directs a current of water into the nest and serves to remove debris and provide oxygen to the developing embryos. The amount of fanning increases over the 7 days it takes for the eggs to hatch. It has been shown that CO2, which is produced by the eggs, is one of the stimuli releasing fanning, and the amount of CO2 produced is greater from older eggs. Thus, one might expect that the increased fanning is a direct effect of CO2 concentration. This supposition was tested in an experiment by Van Iersel (1953). He replaced the old eggs on day 4 with newly laid eggs from another nest. There was a slight drop in fanning with the new eggs, but fanning remained much higher than the original day-1 level. Further, the peak of fanning activity was reached the day the original eggs would have hatched. This means that the stimuli from the eggs must prime a coordinating mechanism and that the state of the coordinating mechanism is no longer completely dependent on stimulation from the eggs after 3 or 4 days.

      A similar example is provided by the development of ovulation in doves. A female ring dove (Streptopelia risoria) will normally lay an egg if she is paired with an acceptable male for about seven days. If the male is removed after 2 or 3 days, the developing egg regresses and is not laid. However, if the male is allowed to remain with the female for 5 days before he is removed, the majority of females will lay an egg 2 days later. Experiments by Lehrman (1965) and his colleagues demonstrated that it is the stimuli from the courting male that prime the mechanism responsible for ovulation.

      Longer-term effects of stimuli can be seen in the yearly cycle of gonad growth and regression in some birds and fish as a result of changes in day length. And changes in day length can also stimulate a host of other physiological changes including those that prepare migratory birds for their long-distance flight (e.g., Piersma & Van Gils 2011) or various mammals for hibernation in the winter (Nelson 2016).

      Hormones and other substances

      Hormones are substances released by endocrine glands into the bloodstream; many of them are known to have behavioral effects. Lashley (1938) suggested that hormones could affect behavior in at least four different ways: during the development of the nervous system, by effects on peripheral structures through alteration of their sensitivity to stimuli, by effects on specific parts of the central nervous system (central behavior mechanisms), and by nonspecific central effects. Abundant evidence for all these modes of action has accumulated since Lashley’s time, although the mechanisms by which hormones influence behavior have turned out to be more complex and diverse than early investigators


Скачать книгу