Child Psychology. Jean-Pascal Assailly

Child Psychology - Jean-Pascal Assailly


Скачать книгу
because, in this case, the child will no longer confide in them, and it is this absence of confidence that will become the causal factor of risky behavior. This is an environmental transaction and parents must be careful, in their words and actions, so as not to block their child’s communication with them.

      Employment rates of mothers have increased substantially in recent decades, with this increase being particularly noticeable among mothers of young children under the age of five. Much of this growth occurred in the 1970s. However, the mid-1990s saw an increase in the employment rate of low-income single mothers.

      The issue of mothers returning to work or entering the workforce shortly after childbirth has been a source of great concern to parents, policymakers and researchers and has prompted numerous analyses of the impact of early maternal employment on child development.

      Developmental psychologists first voiced their concerns in the 1980s in a series of highly discussed and debated papers by Belsky (1988, 1990). In these papers, he highlighted that more than 20 hours per week of out-of-home day care poses risks to the infant–mother relationship and to psychological and behavioral adjustment during infancy, preschool and early elementary school.

      It was against this backdrop of such dramatic changes in motherhood, employment, child care and a heated debate about its effects on child development and on society in general that a National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) and an Early Child Care Research Network were established in the United States.. This network centered its work on the question of whether early child care before the age of five was associated with risks or benefits to child development.

      Together, these views suggest that mothers’ entry into the workforce when their children are young poses particular challenges to their development. Consequently, the trade-off between maternal employment, particularly in the first year of a child’s life, and the effects on children are important empirical questions.

      Literature on the subject (Im 2018) suggests that young children in low-income settings are affected by maternal employment and that these effects differ according to paternal involvement; maternal employment in the early years is associated with adverse effects on aggressive behavior and internalized problems in young children.

      Young children whose mothers begin or return to work in the first year after childbirth thus present internalized problems. Non-maternal care and maternal employment are certainly not identical but are assumed to be related.

      Alternatively, the effect of early maternal employment on a young child’s cognitive functioning may vary according to subgroup-specific characteristics, such as family income. Put another way, poverty could have a pre-weighted effect on the development of cognitive abilities. For example, there are many hypotheses that affluent parents talk much more with their children and use a wider vocabulary. Thus, for economically disadvantaged families, having mothers with their children may result in a weaker impact on cognition if children are still not receiving the stimulation (e.g. expressive and receptive language skills) that predicts later cognitive outcomes, even with their mothers.

      According to the economic view of child development, the main idea was therefore that mothers’ entry into the labor market would increase family economic resources, which would facilitate children’s development, but would require a trade-off between increased income and reduced time spent with children. However, the work of psychologists does not support this hypothesis.

      Rather, this work shows that mothers staying home to care for their infants in the first year, in conjunction with greater father involvement with young children, is a more beneficial solution than bringing more income into the household: children would thrive when high-quality parenting is available, even when the family is financially disadvantaged, and even though high income may facilitate good parenting.

      The EDEN longitudinal study analyzed a number of influences of child care arrangement (Gomajee et al. 2017).

      1.12.1. Child care and emotional and relational development

      Compared with children in informal care, those in group care had a lower likelihood of developing emotional and peer relationship disorders, whereas children in child care (maternal assistant) had a higher likelihood of developing behavioral disorders.

      Children in group child care also had a greater likelihood of developing prosocial behavior. Children who, in particular, spent at least one year in group child care had the most beneficial effect, with girls benefitting more than boys. However, this association was not found among children from disadvantaged families.

      Group child care thus seems to be linked to a protective effect against symptoms of emotional and peer relationship disorders and enables children to develop more prosocial behaviors, but not among children from disadvantaged backgrounds.

      Group child care for young children can therefore have long-term benefits for their behavioral and emotional development, especially if it is of high quality and lasts at least one year.

      1.12.2. Child care and language

      Few studies have described how different infant care experiences may be related to later cognitive, language and motor function, with most analyses focusing on samples from industrialized countries. A Chilean study (Narea 2020) analyzed cognitive, language, motor and vocabulary sub-scores from tests of 7,564 24- to 48-month-old children from the Chilean Early Childhood Longitudinal Survey, which were compared on the basis of retrospective reports of child care arrangements during infancy.

      Compared with those cared for by their mothers, children cared for in centers or by grandparents had higher total cognitive and language sub-scores, while those cared for by grandparents also had higher motor sub-scores. In contrast, children in non-relative care had lower vocabulary scores.

      So to conclude:

       – group child care is positively associated with the child’s global and linguistic development;

       – grandparent care is positively associated with the child’s global, linguistic and motor development;

       – non-parental care is negatively associated with the vocabulary of children;

       – household poverty attenuates the association between non-maternal modes and child development.

      1.12.3. Child care arrangements, academic success and gender

      Gender differences in academic achievement have long been a societal and


Скачать книгу