The Handbook of Solitude. Группа авторов

The Handbook of Solitude - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
and mental depletion affect provision of a safe haven and secure base to a romantic partner. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 31, 630–650.

      63 Modell, A.H. (1993). The Private Self. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.

      64 Overall, N.C., Simpson, J.A., & Struthers, H. (2013). Buffering attachment‐related avoidance: softening emotional and behavioral defenses during conflict discussions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 854–871.

      65 Peplau, L.A. & Perlman, D. (1982, Eds.) Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research, and Therapy. New York: Wiley.

      66 Pepping, C.A., MacDonald, G., & Davis, P.J. (2018). Toward a psychology of singlehood: an attachment‐theory perspective on long‐term singlehood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 324–331.

      67 Pereira, M.G., Taysi, E., Orcan, F., & Fincham, F. (2014). Attachment, infidelity, and loneliness in college students involved in a romantic relationship: the role of relationship satisfaction, morbidity, and prayer for partner. Contemporary Family Therapy: An International Journal, 36, 333–350.

      68 Ringstad, S. & Pepping, C. A. (2016, June). Attachment and long‐term singlehood. Poster presented at the 8th World Congress of Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, Melbourne, Australia.

      69 Rubenstein, C. & Shaver, P.R. (1982). The experience of loneliness. In L.A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research, and Therapy (pp. 206–223). New York: Wiley.

      70 Schachner, D.A., Shaver, P.R., & Gillath, O. (2008). Attachment style and long‐term singlehood. Personal Relationships, 15, 479–491.

      71  Segal, J. (1997). A motivational multidimensional model of approach and avoidance toward togetherness and aloneness. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation. Ramat Gan, Israel: Bar‐Ilan University.

      72 Shallcross, S.L., Frazier, P.A., & Anders, S.L. (2014). Social resources mediate the relations between attachment dimensions and distress following potentially traumatic events. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61, 352–362.

      73 Shaver, P.R. & Hazan, C. (1987). Being lonely, falling in love: perspectives from attachment theory. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 2, 105–124.

      74 Shaver, P.R. & Hazan, C. (1993). Adult romantic attachment: theory and evidence. In D. Perlman & W. Jones (Eds.), Advances in Personal Relationships (Vol. 4, pp. 29–70). London, UK: Kingsley.

      75 Shaver, P.R. & Mikulincer, M. (2002). Attachment‐related psychodynamics. Attachment and Human Development, 4, 133–161.

      76 Spielmann, S.S., Maxwell, J.A., MacDonald, G., & Baratta, P.L. (2013). Don't get your hopes up: avoidantly attached individuals perceive lower social reward when there is potential for intimacy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 219–236.

      77 Sroufe, L.A. & Waters, E. (1977). Attachment as an organizational construct. Child Development, 48, 1184–1199.

      78 Tidwell, M., Reis, H.T., & Shaver, P.R. (1996). Attachment styles, attractiveness, and emotions in social interactions: a diary study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 729–745.

      79 Tuber, S. (2008). Attachment, Play, and Authenticity: A Winnicott Primer. New York: Jason Aronson.

      80 Van Petegem, S., Vansteenkiste, M., & Beyers, W. (2013). The jingle–jangle fallacy in adolescent autonomy in the family: In search of an underlying structure. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42, 994–1014.

      81 Wegner, R., Roy, A.R.K., Gorman, K.R., & Ferguson, K. (2018). Attachment, relationship communication style and the use of jealousy induction techniques in romantic relationships. Personality and Individual Differences, 129, 6–11.

      82 Weiss, R.S. (1973). Loneliness: The experience of emotional and social isolation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

      83 Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. (2006). Nostalgia: content, triggers, functions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 975–993.

      84 Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & & Cordaro, F. (2010). Nostalgia as a repository of social connectedness: the role of attachment‐related avoidance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 573–586.

      85 Winnicott, D.W. (1958). The capacity to be alone. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 39, 416–420.

      86 Winnicott, D.W. (1963). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment: studies in the theory of emotional development. London: Hogarth Press.

      87 Zeifman, D.M. & Hazan, C. (2016). Pair‐bonds as attachments: mounting evidence in support of Bowlby's hypothesis. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications (3rd edition, pp. 416–434). New York: Guilford Press.

      Selin Zeytinogluand Nathan A. Fox

      University of Maryland, USA

      Humans are often described as “social animals” (Batson, 1990). Most animals, however, are social in that they interact with other conspecifics on a regular basis. For example, nonhuman mammals, such as primates and rodents, are both typically reared by their primary caregiver/s in the early stages of their development and continue to interact socially with other conspecifics across their lifetime. Therefore, primate and rodent models offer important opportunities for examining how, why, and when social interactions or their absence matter for healthy development (Brett et al., 2015; Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). One experimental approach used for answering these questions is depriving animals of specific types of social experiences, such as interactions with mothers or peers, and examining the effects of the social deprivation on development. This work is suitable for answering questions such as: (1) How does maternal, paternal, and/or peer deprivation during specific developmental periods affect development?; (2) Does the developmental timing and duration of social deprivation affect developmental outcomes?; and (3) Can the effects of social deprivation or isolation be reversed by later social interactions or enrichment? Another approach examines whether ordinary variations in animal temperament, particularly fearful responses to unfamiliar social partners, are associated with social isolation and health problems.

      In this chapter, we first discuss why it is important to understand the effects of social deprivation and social isolation on humans. Second, we provide a brief review of the theoretical and empirical work on the importance of social relationships in development. Third, we describe the utility of animal models for understanding the potential impact of certain types of social deprivation and isolation experienced by some humans focusing on two main animal models: nonhuman primates and rodents. For each model, we describe the similarities of findings with these species with humans and discuss the unique strengths of research conducted with that population. We then review the nonhuman primate and rodent empirical work on (1) how social deprivation and social isolation as “induced” by experimental manipulations affect development, and (2) how “naturally occurring” variations in temperament, particularly behavioral inhibition, may serve as a risk factor that leads to social isolation and health problems. In conclusion, we discuss the implications of this work for future research directions with humans.


Скачать книгу