Introducing Cognitive Analytic Therapy. Anthony Ryle

Introducing Cognitive Analytic Therapy - Anthony  Ryle


Скачать книгу
of neural pathways which occur during early development. To quote a classic review of the field by Eisenberg (1995): “Major brain pathways are specified by the genome; detailed connections are fashioned by, and consequently reflect, socially mediated experience in the world.”

      Four million years ago, our ancestors the australopithecines already shared food and labor and formed nuclear family structures. One and a half million years ago, Homo erectus, blessed with a much larger brain, managed to build shelters, use fire, and develop better tools. Over the following period the size of the brain compared to that of other mammals continued to increase markedly, with a last period of rapid growth occurring 0.3 million years ago. These changes were accompanied by another significant anatomical development: the evolution of the human vocal tract, with its capacity for the rapid generation of differentiated sounds allowing speech.

      Donald describes how contemporary chimpanzees are capable of flexible and non‐stereotypical ways of thinking and relating and how their social organization is dependent on their capacity to remember “large numbers of distinctly individual learned dyadic relationships.” The development of the human brain from an equivalent level went through a number of intermediate stages, each conveying greater cognitive and social advantages. During the first of these (the Mimetic culture), non‐linguistic skills in representing, differentiating, rehearsing, and communicating were elaborated. Knowledge could now be contained and communicated using metaphoric activities; both tool‐using and sign‐using were established. This allowed the greater cohesion of social groups, which developed complex structures sustained by group rituals. The semantic and social structures that developed over the million or more years of this phase were accompanied by developments in the brain which prepared the way for the addition of symbolic language, but it appears that this developed independently, existing alongside the mimetic modes which persisted and are still a powerful aspect of human communication. The evolution of the larynx and the acquisition of language in the Mythic age provided the individual with the basis for the conscious mobilization of mental capacities. It also enormously enhanced the cohesion and purposefulness of human society by linking, in stories and myths, the guiding values and meanings of the group. The power of oral transmission is illustrated by the account of Australian Aboriginal myth which incorporates accurate descriptions of a terrain, recently identified, which has been under the sea for the past 8,000 years (Tudge, 1996). Another example is provided by the Maori of New Zealand–Aotearoa whose ancestors arrived in a small number of boats. Traditional accounts trace their ancestry of different groups to one or other of these boats and genetic studies have provided confirmation of the groupings.

      Many authors (reviewed in Gilbert, 1992; McGuire & Troisi, 1998; Stevens & Price, 1996) suggest that pre‐programmed patterns, analogous to those triggered by the “innate releasing mechanisms” described by ethologists, may underlie our tendency to think and act in certain ways in certain circumstances. The Jungian concept of archetypes can be seen similarly. While requiring careful attention as partial, possible determinants of human behavior, we consider that to exaggerate their importance can be as reductive and misleading as some of the attempts by earlier socio‐biologists to explain culture in terms of the enactment of “hard‐wired” biological tendencies. However, according to these writers, there are highly stereotyped, ritual behaviors seen throughout the animal kingdom associated with, for example, aggression, status‐seeking, mating, or care‐eliciting and care‐giving. The power and apparent “irrationality” of such responses is well exemplified by the experience of falling in love or the dedicated preoccupation of a nursing mother with her baby. Gilbert (1992) has described the predisposition to enact such phylogenetically evolved “biosocial goals” as “mentalities.” This concept combines affects, action tendencies, and cognitive and attentional structures. These are manifest in social life from early on and could be seen as analogous to or contributing to the formation of RRPs.

      The behavioral patterns (for example care‐ or proximity‐seeking behavior) described by attachment theorists can also be seen to be subsumed within such repertoires. However, as pointed out by Gilbert (1992), they would be, phylogenetically, only one of many adaptive developmental behaviors. Attachment theorists (Bowlby, 1988) have also properly pointed to the life‐long importance of negotiation of issues relating to attachment and loss. In parallel, writers such as Stevens and Price (1996) have described the concept of “frustration of archetypal intent,” by analogy with the ethological phenomenon of the “search for the object never known.” This could manifest, for example, in the case of someone who never had the experience of a good mother or father, as a life‐long search for this never‐experienced, perhaps idealized, relationship. This phenomenon can be recognized clinically and described in terms of role enactments and can be important to identify and work with.

      Конец ознакомительного фрагмента.

      Текст предоставлен ООО «ЛитРес».

      Прочитайте эту книгу целиком, Скачать книгу