A Companion to Chomsky. Группа авторов

A Companion to Chomsky - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
– and which are enduring discoveries in their own right – were observed to apply to some nonlocal dependencies but not others. It was quickly demonstrated that, for any nonlocal dependency D, the syntactic type of the elements involved in the dependency, whether the dependency includes a gap or not (recall (1) versus (3)), and the syntactic position occupied by the top member of the dependency can each determine what constraints are emplaced on successfully establishing D in the first place.

      For instance, dependencies between syntactic heads of phrases (e.g. (10)) are more constrained than dependencies between syntactic phrases themselves, as the former can only be established across a very short structural distance (two structurally consecutive syntactic heads; Travis 1984), whereas certain dependencies between syntactic phrases can be unbounded. With regards to dependencies between phrases, those involving gaps as tail members typically cannot be established if the gap is contained in one of a handful of distinguished phrases known as islands (Ross 1967), whereas those without gaps are typically untroubled by islands (compare (11) and (12), with an “adjunct island” headed by because). Therefore, the status of a dependency's tail also determines across what distance the dependency can be established.

      1 (10) Will Polly Δ be attending the party?

      2 (11) Every auntie thought that baby Bobby smiled [ISLAND because she had just arrived].

      3 (12) * I know who baby Bobby smiled [ISLAND because Δ had just arrived].

      When there are no syntactic islands present in a sentence, it appears at first glance that the distance across which a dependency with a phrasal tail gap can be established is unrestricted. In the sentences in (13), for instance, the apparent tops and tails of each dependency occupy different clauses, and the dependencies themselves extend across multiple clausal boundaries (which are denoted by C1, C2, and C3).

      1 (13)Xavier seems [C1 to appear [C2 to want [C3 to be hired Δ by Sue]]].Who does Bo think [C1 Zoë hopes [C2 Jo believes [C3 that Sue hired Δ]]]?

      1 (14)

      2 (15)WenglaubstduwenPetermeintwenSusiheiratet?(Felser 2004 (10))whobelieveyouwhoPeterthinkswhoSusimarries‘Who do you believe Peter thinks that Susi is marrying?’

      1 (16)a.Fiona seems to her mother Δ to be a genius.[A](acceptable when Fiona and her are interpreted as referring to the same person)b.*Who does her mother love Δ?[A′](unacceptable when who and her are interpreted as referring to the same person)

      2 (17)a.This secret file should be burned Δ after reading {it / *ΔP}.[A]b.Which secret file did the FBI agent burn Δ after reading {it / ΔP}?[A′]

      3 (18)

      1 (19) Polly Δ often eat‐s raw carrots for breakfast.

      1 (20)ΔAyşekim‐iöp‐tü?Ayşewho‐ACCkiss‐PST‘Who did Ayşe kiss?’

      2 (21)

      In addition, generative linguists have also discovered that some languages, such as Adyghe (a Northwest Caucasian language; see Potsdam and Polinsky 2012), utilize top‐gap A‐dependencies. In (22), the subject of root clause is an unpronounced gap, yet this gap co‐refers with the demonstrative pronoun a‐xe‐me “these/they” in the embedded infinitival clause.

      1 (22)Δ[ a‐xe‐mepj&ip.schwa;sme‐ra‐tx&ip.schwa;‐new ]ø‐fjež'a‐&ip.rscpi;e‐xDEM‐PL‐ERGletter‐ABS3PL.ERG‐write‐INF3ABS‐begin‐PST‐3PL.ABS‘They


Скачать книгу