Introducing Second Language Acquisition. Kirsten M. Hummel

Introducing Second Language Acquisition - Kirsten M. Hummel


Скачать книгу
recent approach. While there is evidence that the model may account for specific aspects of L1 learning such as irregular past tense forms, as indicated in the preceding paragraph, the connectionist model is still in the early stages of testing. One specific critique is that it is not clear how connectionism can account for the effect of nonlinguistic, contextual information, such as tone of voice, gestures, etc., in language acquisition (Tomasello, cited in Paradis 2004). Chapter 5 will discuss emergentist approaches with regard to their relevance to L2 acquisition.

      There are of course other theoretical views on language acquisition, but the ones discussed briefly in this section can be considered as particularly relevant for the examination of second language acquisition that we will undertake in the following chapters.

      There are important differences characterizing the L1 and L2 acquisition situations. In this section, we will point out some dimensions along which learning a second language and learning a first language can be distinguished.

       object permanence

      The understanding that an infant gains during the latter part of the first year that objects continue to exist even though they may no longer be visible.

      On the other hand, L2 learners, by definition learning the target language beyond infancy and early childhood, have already gone through a number of fundamental cognitive stages. Basic cognitive concepts such as the notion of object permanence and means‐end awareness have been attained. In addition, their cognitive development allows them metalinguistic awareness, meaning that they are able to reflect on language as a tool for thought or learning. Due to this awareness, L2 learners come to the language learning task equipped in a very different way from the L1 acquirer. As one example, some L2 learners may and often do prefer to learn through explicit exposure to the grammatical rules of the target language. Individuals with an analytical approach to learning may prefer to use logical reasoning skills to acquire parts of the L2. In L1 acquisition, young children do not yet have the same metalinguistic capacity. They are often incapable of recognizing that words are arbitrary labels for objects. Ask a toddler to suspend her intuitions and call her pet cat “dog” and you are likely to be greeted with puzzled incomprehension. Or try to get a two‐year‐old to focus on the form of his words in order to use the correct past tense form when he is not at the stage for doing so and his metalinguistic shortcomings will probably become rapidly evident. In the following dialogue Tommy ignores his mother's correction:

       metalinguistic awareness

      Ability to reflect on language as an object.

      Tommy:

      “Daddy goed.”

      Mother:

      “No, Tommy, Daddy went to work.”

      Tommy:

      “Daddy goed work.”

      Language learning in practice

      In his entertaining book The tipping point: How little things can make a big difference (2000), Malcolm Gladwell reports that during an episode of Sesame Street, when the character Big Bird changes his name to “Roy” because he does not like the fact that his name describes him physically (a “big bird”), the producers discovered children watching the episode appeared to fail to understand the plot: they were unable to attach another name to Big Bird. Gladwell points out that this illustrates the principle of mutual exclusivity that young children follow – that objects (and big birds) normally only have one label – but it also reveals their metalinguistic limitations, i.e. their inability to reflect on language as an object.

      Another important difference that distinguishes L2 from L1 acquisition is that the older learner already has one linguistic system to fall back on when necessary. This contributes to affective and emotional differences. Learning an L1 is essential to satisfying a person's basic needs for food, as well as ensuring other basic care and security. For the older learner of an L2, one linguistic repertoire is already in place to ensure efficient communication to satisfy basic needs and desires. Furthermore, for the older L2 learner, having to acquire another set of language skills can lead to feelings of alienation from the strongly embedded native language and culture. While the child's crucial first events and emotions are accompanied by communication in the L1, for individuals learning an L2 those deeply embedded feelings are already linked with their native language and as a result similar feelings are less tightly linked to the L2. Related to this


Скачать книгу