Communicating Science in Times of Crisis. Группа авторов

Communicating Science in Times of Crisis - Группа авторов


Скачать книгу
in part due to a fear of social stigma (Lantian et al., 2018), tend to subscribe to a conspiracist worldview. Like any worldview, there are meta-narratives that maintain a coherent sense of the (i) nature of reality, (ii) outgroups, (iii) ingroup, and the role of (iv) self and (v) actions on the (vi) future. Dividing the world into an “us” and a “them” (Leone et al., 2020) and “delineating an enemy” are “to a greater or lesser extent, part of every conspiracy theory” (Madisson, 2014, p. 282). The broad epistemic assumption is that some outgroup(s) facilitate systems and narratives that maintain an illusory image of reality to placate a broad passive and exploited public (“sheep”). This outgroup, influenced by some evil elite or cabal, through intermediary management or agents of enforcement, engages in various potential surveillance and control strategies to maintain the powerlessness of the unsuspecting masses (Huneman & Vorms, 2018).

      Such journeys of exploration and transformation become frameworks for identity development, and thereby become more fully integrated as complex belief and value systems, resistant to subsequent contradiction or reversion to the standard narrative, a “self-sealing quality” of relative immunity to outside rebuttal or counterargument (Sunstein & Vermeule, 2009, p. 207). Such conspiracy theory believers thus become heroic truth seekers, who are more woke and aware than the sheep that populate most of society. The processes involved in this journey in regard to epidemics may also trade in processes of scapegoating and heroization that can be tracked through various media (Atlani-Duault et al., 2020).

      Of Narratives, Stories, and Theories

      Theories are constituted of metaphors (Hawes, 1975) and narratives (DiMaggio, 1995; Pentland, 1999; Shepherd & Suddaby, 2017), so one tributary of scholarly inquiry and theory needs to understand what makes narratives cognitively and memetically “sticky” (Stano, 2020). Stories and narratives represent one of the fundamental ways in which people make sense of and communicate about policies (Peterson, 2018). The nature of narratives (Corrigan & Denton, 1996), and therefore, conspiracy theories (Gebauer et al., 2016), is to provide descriptive and explanatory arcs of actors and events, which naturally fit into human conceptions of causation (Corrigan & Denton, 1996). “Because of their explanatory power, stories can be linked into cycles to form conspiracy theories, often bringing together normally disparate domains of human interaction into a single, explanatory realm” (Shahsavari et al., 2020, p. 3). For example, a study of anti-vax Instagram posts found that they had greater engagement, more misinformation, and were more likely to involve personal narratives, compared to provax posts (Kearney et al., 2019).

      Fisher’s (1980, 1985a, 1985b) narrative paradigm, which begins with the ontological shift of viewing humans as homo narrans, suggests that rhetorical competence consists of the rationality involved in the “mastery of the logic of good reasons,” which insures “the minimal, perhaps the optimal, kind of knowledge that must inform the invention, composition, presentation, and criticism of rhetorical messages and interactions” (Fisher, 1980, p. 122). There are two primary criteria of narrative rationality: narrative fidelity (i.e., “the soundness of its reasoning and the value of its values,” Fisher, 1985a, pp. 349–350) and narrative probability (i.e., the extent to which “a story coheres or ‘hangs together,’ whether or not the story is free of contradictions,” p. 349). Each of these criteria can be further differentiated (Baesler, 1995). Conspiracy theory and much of the fake news that provide fuel for such theory are rhetorically shaped to appeal to narrative fidelity and probability in the two genres that support such narrative structures: “storytelling and argumentation” (Bangerter et al., 2020, p. 207).

      Given a reservoir of cultural stories and mythic narrative structures from which to draw, “conspiracy theorists collaboratively negotiate a single explanatory narrative framework, often composed of a pastiche of smaller narratives, aligning otherwise unaligned domains of human interaction as they develop a totalizing narrative” (Shahsavari et al., 2020, p. 16). These narrativizing predispositions can be illustrated even in early representations of disease outbreaks. For example, in considering the records of the fourteenth-century Black Death pandemic, Carmichael (1998) concluded that “most plague accounts … impose narrative order on a past plague, assigning its beginning, middle, and end, and selecting which facts and memories are needed to capture the essence or meaning of the plague” (p. 134).

      Such narrative configurations or templates are stoked by routine and evolved cognitive biases that enhance narrative fidelity. For example, there is some experimental evidence for a Sarrazin effect, in which the greater the availability or accessibility of extreme or ordinarily implausible explanatory options, the more people tend to move toward conspiratorial accounts (Raab et al., 2013). Another conjecture is that conspiratorial thinking is reinforced by a tendency to seek “us-versus-them” identities (Leone et al., 2020). A third cognitive bias may be the tendency to attribute or seek intention (Brotherton & French, 2015) or agency (Douglas et al., 2016) underlying otherwise potentially accidental or coincidental events, which may also take the form of a tendency of people to presume greater explanatory depth, precision, and coherence than actually exists (Rozenblit & Keil, 2002). There is also evidence for the proportionality bias, which predicts that extreme events cannot be the product of nonextreme causes, and thus, vast disruptive and unusual events stimulate human tendencies to attribute agency, to concoct narratives of actors pulling levers behind the curtain, simply, for example, because “in medical conspiracy theories, unfortunate things (such as, say, the outbreak of some virus) cannot just happen without a purpose” (Andrade, 2020, p. 5). Furthermore, “the more horrendous the consequences of an event, the more brutal and inhuman are those who caused it” (Madisson, 2014, p. 297). In short, narratives with anthropomorphized actors can provide coherence to the unimaginable incomprehensibility of random chance and evolutionary biology.

      An iconic-continuous originator of danger is depicted, to a greater or lesser extent, in all conspiracy theories, … where it is expressed in approximately this


Скачать книгу