Small Teaching. James M. Lang
Reflection. As Daniel Willingham has argued, “Memory is the residue of thought” (Willingham 2014, p. 54). In other words, we remember what we spend a little time thinking about. Prediction provides an excellent spur for thought, in that you can ask students to think about why they made their prediction, what actually happened (if the prediction leads to direct observation), and why their prediction was right or wrong. If you are asking students at the beginning of a class to write down a prediction and having a few of them read their predictions aloud, return to those students at the end of class and ask them to explain why they made those predictions. Students who made correct predictions can be asked to articulate the principle or concept that helped them get it right; students who made incorrect predictions can repair their understanding by articulating the correct ideas.
SMALL TEACHING QUICK TIPS: PREDICTING
Predictive activities are the ideal starting point for a small teaching approach, because they are so easy to slot into the opening and closing of a learning unit. These reliable prediction activities give you some practical starting points.
Open the course with a class brainstorming activity in which you ask students to surface their prior knowledge. Give them time individually or in small groups, and then work as a class to organize that knowledge in ways that will set up the learning to come.
At the beginning of the class, unit, or course, give students a brief pretest on the material. For example, give an opening-week pretest that is similar in format to the final exam.
Use classroom polling to break up your course lectures and ask students questions about the next topic you will cover. Pose the question, have them respond with whatever poll technology you are using, and then invite some paired or whole-class discussion of their responses before you move into your explanation.
When presenting cases, problems, examples, or histories, stop before the conclusion and ask students to predict the outcome. Invite them to reflect afterward on why they got it wrong or right. In other words: pause, predict, ponder.
Close class by asking students to make predictions about material that will be covered in the next class session.
CONCLUSION
As I was finishing the second edition of this book, an article appeared in a psychology journal which provided another plank of support for the power of prediction, as well as one more reason why prediction has such a positive impact (Brod 2021). The article describes an experiment in which the researchers asked students to predict the scores of a soccer game, then revealed the correct scores to them, and afterward tested their memory of those scores. But they compared the students who engaged in these predictive activities to another group of students who did something slightly different: they were shown the correct scores, and then asked to explain whether those scores matched their expectations about them. In other words, the students in that second group were essentially being asked: If you had made a prediction, would it have been correct? As you might expect from the research I have provided throughout this chapter, the students who made the actual predictions had better memory for the soccer games than did the students who only reflected afterward upon how well the scores matched their expectations. The concrete act of making the prediction was the crucial differentiator: they had to use their prior knowledge of soccer to hazard a prediction, and then see the results afterward.
The researchers in this study added one interesting element to their experiment, though. They looked at the pupil dilation of the subjects as the answer was revealed to them. When the subjects who had made predictions saw the correct answers, their pupils dilated, indicating surprise. The researchers thus argue that one of the driving mechanisms behind the learning power of prediction is the quick emotional burst we get from seeing how our prediction turns out. As we shall discuss further in Chapter Eight, emotions can play a key role in enhancing our learning. In this experiment, the predictions that the subjects made about the soccer games heightened their emotional investment in finding out the scores, and this emotional investment paid dividends in their learning. This finding on the emotional impact of prediction led the researchers to conclude that random guessing doesn't have the same impact as prediction. When I make a random guess, I'm aware of its randomness, and don't make an emotional commitment to it. When I make a prediction, I'm drawing on my prior knowledge to try to understand a novel situation, and as a result I become more emotionally invested in the outcome: I want to know if I was right. It's that emotional investment, the researchers argue, that makes the difference, and that helps lodge the new knowledge firmly in my brain.
Keep this finding in mind as you are engineering predictive activities for your students. If you give them a pretest, encourage them to draw on their previous knowledge to try and get as many correct as possible. If you are asking students to predict something in class with electronic polling, actually give them the time they would need to think about it and make that prediction. The more they commit themselves to their predictions, the more emotion they will feel at the revelation of the correct answer—and the more they are likely to remember it in the future.
Конец ознакомительного фрагмента.
Текст предоставлен ООО «ЛитРес».
Прочитайте эту книгу целиком, купив полную легальную версию на ЛитРес.
Безопасно оплатить книгу можно банковской картой Visa, MasterCard, Maestro, со счета мобильного телефона, с платежного терминала, в салоне МТС или Связной, через PayPal, WebMoney, Яндекс.Деньги, QIWI Кошелек, бонусными картами или другим удобным Вам способом.